Graduate Research Day: Difference between revisions

From PNB Graduate Handbook
Jump to navigation Jump to search
(Replaced content with "Further Information Coming Soon!")
Tag: Replaced
 
(68 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
= Graduate Research Day - February 11, 2021 =
Further Information Coming Soon!
 
Graduate Research Day 2021 will be held on the [https://www.airmeet.com Airmeet.com] platform. <br>
The link to the conference is: 
 
 
== Schedule ==
=== 9:30-10:05 AM Keynote 1: Dr. Mayu Nishimura ===
* Title
=== 10:05-10:50 AM Symposium 1 ===
==== Tovah Kashetsky ====
<p style="margin-left:40px">Effect of Experience on Collective Decision-Making and Social Organization <br>
''Tovah Kashetsky, Grant Doering, and Reuven Dukas''<br></p>
 
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Expertise built from experience allows individuals to perform significantly better than novices on a complex task. Social groups can also demonstrate expertise. Within social groups, collective decision-making is crucial for maintaining cohesion, but it is unknown whether a group’s collective decision-making skills can improve with experience. To investigate this, we tested whether repeated experience with choosing between multiple nests during emigration in house-hunting ants (Temnothorax ambiguus) would improve the speed and efficiency with which colonies reach consensus. We hypothesize that experience with decision-making would improve colony performance on future decisions. We first ran preliminary experiments to quantify nest features that colonies prefer in order to construct artificial nests of varying attractiveness. We will provide 20 colonies experience with a choice between a good- and poor-quality nest during emigration, and 20 colonies with no choice during emigration (a single nest). Lastly, we will test all colonies to decide between a good- and poor-quality nest during a final emigration. So far, we found that colonies with experience decision-making do indeed appear to be faster and more efficient at decisions than colonies without experience decision-making. We will also run a social network analysis on 3 colonies from both groups to examine temporal changes in social organization. This will provide us with a mechanistic explanation for how improvements in collective decision-making arise from the actions of individuals. Studying decision-making in ants will allow us to achieve an improved understanding of the development and mechanisms behind expertise.</p>
 
==== Hanna Haponenko ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Depth-specific IOR effect when attention shifts from far to near space relative to viewer <br>
''Hanna Haponenko, Hong Jin Sun <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px">
Inhibition of return (IOR) is a phenomenon where responses to a peripheral target are delayed if the target appears more than 300ms in the same location as a previous cue. IOR has been extensively shown to operate in 2D scenes. It is not fully understood whether IOR is determined by relative location between cue and target in retinal coordinates or world coordinates. Such a question can be studied by examining IOR in 3D scenes. We compared IOR when cues and targets appeared at same or different depth planes and when depth information was provided by monocular cues. When the cue and target appeared at different depths, a vertical offset was created on-screen, a potential confound to depth perception. We removed the contribution of this confound by contrasting the 3D condition with a 2D control condition that matched cue and target positions but removed all context simulating 3D space. Results showed that IOR magnitude decreased for the different-depth condition compared to the same-depth condition in 3D displays. IOR magnitude also decreased as a function of vertical offset in corresponding 2D displays. Most importantly, such magnitude reduction in 3D displays was higher than that in the 2D displays, but only when the difference in depth was caused by the target appearing at a nearer position compared to the cue. We thus have identified a depth-specific IOR effect in a setting strictly comprised of monocular depth cues, which occurs only when attention shifts from far to near space relative to the viewer. </p>
 
==== Joanna Spyra ====
<p style="margin-left:40px;">
Memory for global musical structures: Dissecting musical features for their contribution to memory for nonadjacent tonal centers <br>
''Joanna Spyra & Dr. Matthew Woolhouse'' <br> </p>
 
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:12px"> Memory for musical keys is exceptionally poor. Studies have found that participants maintain a memory for key for only 11-20 seconds after key-change occurs. But music is a complex stimulus with many features; how do these features, such as rhythmical activity or timbre, contribute to the maintenance of memory for past musical sequences? In the Digital Music Lab, we employ a paradigm called “nonadjacent key relationships” to tease apart these musical features and examine their unique effects on memory for key. This paradigm divides stimuli into three sections: (1) a key-defining nonadjacent section, (2) an intervening section in a different key, and (3) a probe cadence either in the original key or in a third key (forming an ABA or CBA relationship between the three sections). Participants are asked to rate the probe for its goodness-of-completion, the idea being that if a memory for the original key remains—despite intervening information—participants will rate the ABA condition higher than the CBA condition. Using this as a baseline, we can manipulate various musical features and compare the strength of completion ratings. If a feature boosts memory, goodness-of-completion should receive a similar boost when compared to CBA conditions. Indeed, this is a pattern we found in many musical features. Results confirm that though memory for key itself may be weak, it is supported by common features we use in music composition every day. </p>
 
=== 10:50–11:00 AM Break 1 ===
=== 11:00-11:45 AM Symposium 2 ===
==== Emily Wood ====
 
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Body sway reflects nonverbal communication in a string quartet learning to play unfamiliar music together <br>
''Emily Wood, Dobri Dotov, Andrew Chang, Dan Bosnyak, Lucas Klein & Laurel Trainor <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Ensemble musicians must anticipate their partners’ actions to coordinate playing a piece together. To achieve this, musicians attend to sensorimotor signals embedded in their partners’ body sway movements. Indeed, a musician’s body sway movements reveal their upcoming intent regarding phrasing, tempo, and dynamics, which helps their partners anticipate how and when to play next. We have previously measured the body sway of expert musicians in small ensembles with motion capture, and used Granger Causality (GC) to calculate bidirectional influence, or information flow, between the body sway of each musician in the ensemble. We showed that information flow was greater from assigned leaders to assigned followers than vice versa, and that group information flow was greater when musicians played with emotional expression than without. Here, we show how information flow changes in an ensemble that learns to play unfamiliar music together. A professional string quartet came into the LIVELab and played two unfamiliar pieces of music together eight times in succession while body sway motion data was recorded. Linear mixed effect modelling showed that information flow within the group decreased significantly across trials for both pieces, suggesting that musicians relied on body sway to help them play together when the pieces were most novel (trial 1), but this reliance decreased as they gained familiarity with playing the pieces together. We are currently completing cross-correlation analyses to examine how the similarity of group body sway movements changes across trials. Overall, our studies show that body sway reflects nonverbal communication in musical ensembles. </p>
 
==== Wei (Vivian) Fang ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Dominance pulls faces closer <br>
''Wei Fang, Cristina I. Galusca, Zhe Wang, Yu-Hao Sun, Olivier Pascalis, Naiqi G. Xiao <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Perceived social traits, such as dominance and trustworthiness, affect other people’s behaviors. While the impact of social traits has been consistently found in high-level cognitive processing, it is unclear whether social traits also modulates perceptual processing of faces. To this end, we investigated how facial dominance affects the perceived distance of faces.
 
We used an implicit but highly robust perceptual illusion to measure the perceived distance: when two identical faces are presented vertically (one above the other), relative to the top face, the bottom one appears closer. Observers exhibit a strong bias to indicate the bottom face is bigger.<br>
 
We examined how facial dominance influences the perceived distance with a set of computer-generated Dominant and Submissive faces. If facial dominance makes faces perceived closer, participants will likely report the bottom one is bigger. To probe the generality of this effect, we tested this effect in Canada, China, and France (N = 30/country) with faces from three races (African, Asian, and Caucasian).<br>
 
Across the three countries, participants showed a significant bias in choosing the bottom face as the bigger (Mean bottom responses = 72.03%, p < .001), replicating the illusion. Moreover, Dominant faces led to a stronger illusion than Submissive faces (p = .009), suggesting that facial dominance led faces to be perceived closer. No effect of face race or country were found.<br>
 
As facial dominance is often associated with negative signals, our finding suggests an evolutional mechanism in the visual system, which amplifies dangerous signals in the environment.<br> </p>
 
==== Hannah M. Anderson ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Variation and correlations of behavioral lateralization <br>
''Hannah M. Anderson, David N. Fisher, Brendan L. McEwen, Justin Yeager, Jonathan N. Pruitt, James B. Barnett
<br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Sensory and behavioral laterality, or “handedness,” is widespread across the animal kingdom and is thought to increase neural efficiency and dual information processing. Historically, research on behavioral lateralization has focused on among-individual variation, but the importance of within-individual variation in behavior is being increasingly acknowledged. Among-individual laterality correlations can indicate both neural multitasking or linkage of stimuli and/or behaviors; however, within-individual correlations of lateralization have yet to be explored experimentally. We adopted a multivariate approach to investigate lateralization at both the population and individual level in two species of terrestrial frog: the poison frog Ameerega bilinguis and their Batesian mimic Allobates zaparo. In contrast to other research on the subject we found no evidence for among-individual correlations but did find evidence for within-individual correlations, a previously unexplored form of lateralization. We discuss possible meanings for these results and their broader implications to both lateralization and broader behavioral research. </p>
 
=== 1:00-2:00 PM Symposium 3 ===
==== Leigh Greenberg ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Visualizing critical information for the perception of androgyny
<br>
''Leigh Greenberg, Patrick J. Bennett, Allison B. Sekuler
<br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Androgynous face stimuli typically are generated by morphing strongly masculine and strongly feminine faces, based on an assumption that androgynous faces are equally masculine and feminine. Our past work challenged that assumption, finding that faces could be perceived simultaneously as androgynous and strongly gendered. The current study uses a reverse correlation technique (Dotsch & Todorov, 2011) to examine the stimulus characteristics that make a face look more or less androgynous. Observers viewed a pair of male or female faces embedded in Gaussian white noise and chose the face that appeared more androgynous. The two noise fields varied across trials, but were anti-correlated within each trial. Noise fields were sorted based on observer responses and averaged to create a Classification Image (CI) and antiCI. Preliminary results showed that the spatial structure in the CI was related to perceived androgyny: when the CI and antiCI were added to the base face images, the base+CI was clearly more androgynous than the base alone or the base+antiCI. Currently, we are investigating the similarity of CIs obtained from different observers and from different base male and female faces. To verify our findings, we plan to have new observers view CI and antiCI pairs and judge which is more androgynous. We also plan to use this technique to create additional CIs that represent concepts related to androgyny, such as masculinity and femininity. The results of these studies will shed light on our understanding of how the visual system processes face gender information.
</p>
 
==== Jesse Pazdera ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Pitch induces illusory percepts of time
<br>
''Jesse K. Pazdera, Laurel J. Trainor
<br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Accurate tempo-tracking in auditory perception helps us to direct our attention to critical moments in speech and music. However, past research suggests that perceived tempo can be influenced by other, nontemporal features of an auditory stimulus. For example, people tend to rate music as faster when it is played in a higher octave than when it is played in a lower octave. It has therefore been suggested that pitch height alters tempo perception. However, previous studies have typically compared only one lower register with one higher register, leaving pitch height confounded with other factors like cochlear sensitivity that also change across octaves. My research this year has focused on mapping out these illusory tempo effects over six octaves to disambiguate these factors and better understand how tempo perception varies across the broader range of human hearing. These findings will help us to develop theories as to why seemingly unrelated acoustic information is capable of affecting time perception, and they may teach us something new about how our brains process time. In this talk I will discuss the results of a few of our latest experiments, as well as our working theory as to how pitch induces illusory percepts of time.
</p>
 
==== Brendan McEwen ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Camouflage, conspicuousness, & antipredator behaviour in an Amazonian poison frog & non-toxic mimic
<br>
''Brendan L. McEwen, Isaac D. Kinley, Hannah M. Anderson, Justin Yeager, Jonathan N. Pruitt, James B. Barnett
<br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> The Amazonian rainforest is a dynamic environment brimming with predation threats, and prey species occupying these habitats are under immense pressure to reduce these pressures on their survival. While some species avoid detection through cryptic strategies, i.e. camouflage, others aim to make their presence known to predators by signaling toxic defenses with bright colouration – an evolutionary strategy known as Aposematism. Conventional wisdom would suggest that camouflage and aposematism are mutually exclusive survival strategies, but emerging evidence suggests that aposematic species may simultaneously be cryptic to predators under certain conditions. Aposematic species’ signaling can be parasitized and co-opted by non-toxic species through Batesian Mimicry, affording predator avoidance to these non-defended species. However, mimics are often imperfect in ways that may affect how conspicuous they are to would-be-predators. Further, even while mimicking a toxic species, mimics are under much greater predation threat than their models and may need to behave differently in the face of danger. We examined differences in conspicuousness between the toxic Amazonian frog Ameerega bilinguis and its non-toxic mimic Allobates zaparo using simulated predator vision and human detectors. We then attempt to associate these differences in detectability to differences in antipredator behaviour trials performed in their natural habitat.  We find that components of body colouration leads the non-toxic mimic A. zaparo to be more conspicuous than its toxic model A. bilinguis to both simulated predator visual systems and human detectors, and find evidence for a behavioural antipredator adaptation in A. zaparo to compensate for its lack of chemical defense.
</p>
 
==== Elizabeth Phillips ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> A vocal-melodic theory of the evolutionary origins of musical scales
<br>
''Elizabeth Phillips, Steven Brown
<br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> The predominant theory of the origin of musical scales stems from the observation that some musical intervals are related to frequency ratios present in the natural harmonic series. From the time of the ancient Greeks onward, harmonicity theory has influenced the practice of instrumental tuning in Western music. However, the voice is the most ancestral and universal pitched instrument and it cannot be pre-tuned the way that instruments can. As a result, sung notes are notoriously imprecise in their tuning properties. In the present study, we analyzed the precision and tuning of notes from vocal melodies – from both European folk and non-Western indigenous traditions – relative to flute melodies of both folk and classical European origin. The results showed that, whereas Western instrumental melodies conformed somewhat to harmonicity, vocal melodies showed significant pitch imprecision and a general disregard for harmonic tuning principles. Importantly, these vocal effects were present regardless of culture, suggesting that they arise from physiological limitations in the vocal-production mechanism. These results highlight striking differences in the pitch-class properties of the voice and musical instruments. Given that song is the most ancestral form of pitched music, these results point to shortcomings in the harmonicity theory and thus call for the development of a vocal-melodic theory of the origin of musical scales. This evolutionary theory should be built cross-culturally, by investigating how the biological- and cultural-evolution mechanisms constraining all vocal music may consistently shape musical scale structure. </p>
 
=== 2:00 - 2:45 PM Poster Session ===
==== Rachael Finnerty ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Online Group Music Therapy: A proactive approach
<br>
''Rachael Finnerty, Laurel Trainor
<br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> This research explores the efficacy of online group music therapy as a proactive intervention for university students to manage stress and anxiety in comparison to the standard of care, verbal counseling. Students are randomly assigned to one of: 1) online active group music therapy, 2) online receptive group music therapy, 3) online group verbal therapy, 4) wait-listed control group, 5) no-intervention control group. Demographic information, music background and the Ten Item Personality Index are collected. Before and after each online therapy session, students rate their stress (Likert scale), complete the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (State) questionnaire and provide heart rate variability using a phone app. The online therapy groups run once a week for six-weeks. All participants complete the WHO-Quality of Life (short-form), the Perceived Stress Scale and provide a hair sample for cortisol analysis before and after the six-week intervention. We expect stress and anxiety to decrease in all three online therapy groups from before to after each session as well as from before to after the six-week intervention, whereas we expect little change in the two control groups. Data collection is ongoing in cohorts of 50 (10 per group), with a goal to test 200 participants. The efficacy of online group music therapy will be compared to the verbal standard of care. If online group music therapy is effective, it could become a relatively low-cost proactive approach to managing stress and anxiety, lowering the strain on health care supports on campuses, and enhancing overall student well-being.
</p>
 
==== Cindy Tran ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Learning conversations in the operating room: The impact of new surgical coaching tool
<br>
''Cindy Tran, Jennifer Zering, Kathleen Howcraft, Ranil Sonnadara
<br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Introduction: Despite evidence positioning coaching techniques as a central component of competency-based medical education (CBME), implementations of coaching are highly heterogenous in medical education to date. In the present study, we explored the impact of a new surgical coaching checklist on teaching and learning experiences in the operating room (OR).
Method: Eight staff and eight surgical trainees used the new coaching checklist for four weeks. Following this, they participated in individual interviews. Questions explored experiences with the coaching tool, barriers and facilitators to use of the tool, and perceived effectiveness of the tool. Interviews were transcribed verbatim and analyzed for themes. <br>
Results: Staff and trainees reported that the checklist was helpful, providing opportunities for persistent interactions, facilitating more direct feedback, and creating a consistent, structured framework for teaching in the OR. The tool was most effective when both parties believed its use would add value to the teaching encounter. Lack of preparation, individual teaching preferences, inability to communicate openly, and competing time demands impeded usage of the tool. <br>
Conclusions: The surgical coaching tool fosters good educational practice and more positive teaching and learning experiences, encouraging a more collaborative teaching environment. The adoption of coaching techniques continues to become increasingly important as more programs shift towards CBME curriculums. Future work will further explore how the tool will help provision and interpretation of meaningful feedback and improving the culture around assessment in CBME.  </p>
 
==== Seyedbehrad Dehnadi ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Exploration of the Bayesian Model of Tactile-Spatial Perception
<br>
''Seyedbehrad Dehnadi, Daniel Goldreich, Jonathan Tong, Lux Li
<br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Our brains are remarkable at producing an accurate perception of the world from the noisy and ambiguous sensory information they receive. The process by which sensory neurons encode physical stimuli as neural signals is relatively well understood. However, much less is known on how the brain infers stimuli from these signals. While it is accepted that this inference is based on a series of assumptions the brain makes about the world, the details of this inference process require further investigation. <br>
A promising computational model of the brain that can optimally explain the inference process is the Bayesian model. This statistical model proposes that perception is a dynamic process that combines stimulus information with our past experiences. While this model has been effective in replicating human performance on visual and auditory-based perceptual decision-making tasks, it has scarcely been applied in studies of tactile perception. As a result, it is not known if the Bayesian model of perception is generalizable to all senses. <br>
My thesis explores whether a Bayesian observer with access to cortical firing rates could replicate human perception on tactile spatial tasks. So far, we have been able to broadly replicate human performance on Adaptation Induced Repulsion Illusion, Classic Two-Point Discrimination task, Two-Point Discrimination task and Classic Two-Point Discrimination task. We are planning to investigate other tactile experiments and derive prediction based on our model. We hope that our model will deepen the basic scientific understanding of perceptual processing and be beneficial in the construction of neuroprosthetics involving tactile perception. </p>
 
==== Konrad Swierczek ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
==== Emma Marsden ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
==== Carly McIntyre-Wood ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
==== Peter Najdzionek ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
==== Maya Flannery ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
==== Jiali Song ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
=== 3:00 - 3:30 PM Lightning Talks ===
==== Vidhi Patel ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
==== Lucas Klein ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
==== Jamie Cochrane ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
==== Janice Yan ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
==== James Mirabelli ====
<p style="margin-left:40px"> Title <br>
''Authors <br>'' </p>
<p style="margin-left:40px;text-indent:40px;font-size:13px"> Abstract Text </p>
 
== Prizes ==
Top three:
*Oral Presentation: $75
*Poster Presentation: $75
*Lightning Talks: $50
<!-----Most Popular Meme will be featured on this page!!----->
Winners to be announced...

Latest revision as of 16:09, 25 August 2022

Further Information Coming Soon!